Thursday 19 June 2014

Discuss the role of genetics into aggression.

(8+16)

AO1 Twin Studies
Monozygotic (MZ) twins share all of their genetic information whilst dizygotic twins (DZ) share 50%. If MZ twins are more alike in terms of aggressive behaviour than DZ twins, then this should be due to genes rather than environment. It is much harder to study aggressive behaviour than criminal behaviour, so few studies have tried to measure aggressive behaviour directly. However, Coccaro did use twins to study aggressive behaviour and found that 50% of the aggression towards to each other could be attributed to genetic factors.


AO2
However one problem when studying twins is that twins will often be brought up together in a similar way, so it is hard to say whether the concordance rates are caused by genetic or environmental factors. To solve this problem psychologists can carry out adoption studies where they observe the levels of aggressive behaviour in adopted children and their biological parents. If there is a high correlation between aggression levels of the two you can say it is genetics not the environment that caused this aggression. Hutchings and Mednick (1975) studied 14,000 adopted children and found that a high proportion of boys with criminal convictions had biological parents with criminal convictions too, suggesting a link between aggression and genetics.

AO2 
There are a number of studies that have used twins. Miles and Carey (1997) looked at 24 studies involving twins or adopted children. They found that genetic influences were very important in these studies. Another meta-analysis carried out by Rhee and Waldman (2002) also found that genetic factors were very important in aggressive behaviour.

IDA
The genetic argument is also supported by real-life examples of violent individuals. Reggie and Ronnie Kray were identical twins who were also violent criminals and were responsible for a series of murders in the 1960s. Robert and Stephen Spahalski were twin brothers in the USA. They were raised together in childhood but separated afterwards. Both became serial killers.

AO1 MAOA 
The Monoamine Oxidase A (MAOA) gene attracted interest when it was found that mice lacking the gene were excessively aggressive. Monoamine oxidase A is an enzyme that breaks down important neurotransmitters in the brain, including dopamine, norepinephrine, and serotonin. The enzyme is regulated by the monoamine oxidase A gene (MAOA). Humans have various forms of the gene, resulting in different levels of enzymatic activity. People with the low-activity form (MAOA-L) produce less of the enzyme, while the high-activity form (MAOA-H) produces more of the enzyme.

AO2
Brunner found evidence that the MAOA deficiency is genetic. Brunner conducted a study on a large Dutch family, in which all the males had a mutant form of the MAOA gene. All had borderline retardation and reacted aggressively when angry, fearful or frustrated, suggesting that abnormal MAOA activity is associated with aggression. Brunner’s research however, is extremely limited as it is a case study, meaning it used a small sample. Although a case study produces data rich in detail, the conclusions cannot necessarily be applied to a wider population. Also, there are other reasons that could explain the disproportionate amount of aggressive males in the family studied; examples being dispositional factors and the pressure of maintaining the family’s reputation.

AO2 
Moffitt also conducted research into the MAOA deficiency. Researchers performed a longitudinal study on 442 NZ males from birth to age 26, recording which participants suffered abuse as children and activity of participant’s MAOA gene. It was found that those who had suffered abuse, and had the deficient MAOA gene were 9x more likely to indulge in antisocial behaviour, including aggression. Participants who had not been abused, but carried the high activity form of MAOA were not more aggressive than anyone else. This suggests that the MAOA gene has some influence on aggression, but it is sensitive to social experiences in early development. This implicates the idea of a diathesis stress model; some people are genetically pre-disposed to be aggressive, but it will only manifest itself when triggered by an environmental experience. Moffitt’s research used a large sample over a long period of time, suggesting that the conclusions made can be applied to the wider population.  However, as the research was conducted in NZ, it may have produced a culture bias. This may mean that the conclusions cannot be applied to people living in different societies.

AO2
However, the impact that genes alone have on aggression has been elusive. It seems that our genes affect us most when combined with certain environmental stimuli e.g. in Moffitt’s research a genetic deficiency was only made apparent when triggered by abuse. This suggests that the genetic approach is reductionist, as it ignores the social and environmental factors that impact on our aggression. It is likely that the interactionist approach proposed by Moffatt is the most valid, as it accounts for genetic factors (MAOA) as well as environmental factors (abuse).

IDA
The approach is also deeply deterministic, in that it states that we are passive victims of our own genes, disregarding our free will in choosing how to behave.  Also, many studies into genetics have used animals, as they have quicker breeding cycles, so intergenerational effects can be observed sooner. This however, presents a variety of ethical issues. For example, animals cannot provide informed consent, and cannot tell us if they have been harmed by the research. Also, it is extremely difficult to generalise conclusions made about the genetics of animals to humans, we are simply too different.

IDA 
Also, measuring aggression in such studies can be difficult. How do we operationalize aggressive acts? For example, in Moffatt’s study, how did researchers determine whether a participant was behaving aggressively or not? This could present a problem of inter-study reliability as measures are not consistent.

IDA

Many of the studies in this area have focused on individuals convicted of violent crime. However, these results only represent a small minority of those who are regularly involved in aggressive behaviour, as many violent attacks do not results in a conviction. These individuals may also not be habitual offenders; they may be usually calm people who were designated as violent for a one-time offense. This may explain why so many studies have found little or no evidence of heritability for violence.

No comments:

Post a Comment